Artículos
Association between
electronic media use, development milestones and language in infants
Asociación entre el
uso de medios electrónicos, hitos del desarrollo y lenguaje en infantes
Maian Stamati maistamati@gmail.com
Universidad Abierta Interamericana, Argentina
Lucas G. Gago-Galvagno lucas.gagogalvagno@hotmail.com
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas
(CONICET), Argentina
Stephanie E. Miller semille5@go.olemiss.edu
University of Mississippi,
Estados
Unidos de América
Angel M. Elgier amelgier@gmail.com
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas
(CONICET), Argentina
Rocío A. Hauché hauche.rocio@gmail.com
Universidad de Buenos Aires,
Argentina
Susana C. Azzollini susana1060@yahoo.com.ar
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas
(CONICET), Argentina
Association between electronic media use, development milestones
and language in infants
Interdisciplinaria,
vol. 39, núm. 3, pp. 151-166,
2022
Centro Interamericano de Investigaciones Psicológicas y Ciencias
Afines
Los autores/as
conservan los derechos de autor y ceden a la revista el derecho de la primera
publicación, con el trabajo registrado con la licencia de atribución, no
comercial, compartir igual de Creative Commons
Esta obra está bajo una Licencia Creative
Commons Atribución-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0 Internacional.
Recepción:
27
Septiembre 2021
Aprobación:
10
Agosto 2022
Abstract:
The use of electronic media has
increased in early childhood. During early childhood, language and motor skills
are important for the development of other cognitive skills. Therefore, it
becomes essential to study how the use of screens is associated with these
important skills in the first years of life. The objective of the following
research was to describe the use of electronic media (i. e., TV, cell phone and
tablet) and its association with language and developmental milestones in the
first years of life. Participants were 253 primary caregivers of infants
between 2 to 48 months (M = 30.17 months, SD = 10.82, female = 124). Parental
reports of infant media use, motor and language development milestones, the
MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI) and the Permanent
Household Survey (EPH) for sociodemographic variables were used. The results
showed that, on average, infants began to use screens during the first year of
life at an average of one hour per day with TV as the most used medium and
video viewing as the activity they did the most. The number of hours of devices
usage was positively associated with lexical density and sentence use.
Furthermore, starting device use at a later age was associated with lower
scores in language reports and later milestones of motor and language
development. No differences were found based on the type of device content and
sociodemographic variables. Results indicate that the excessive use of screens
could affect some early skills, although it is necessary to investigate the
context in which they are used.
Keywords: electronic media,
development milestones, language, caregivers, infants.
Resumen: En los últimos años, el uso de medios electrónicos ha aumentado
en la infancia temprana debido al creciente acceso a las pantallas y a la
situación de aislamiento ocasionado por la pandemia de COVID-19. Por otro lado,
durante la primera infancia, las habilidades de lenguaje y motrices son
fundamentales para el desarrollo de otras habilidades cognitivas, y se asocian
con el rendimiento académico y habilidades sociales posteriores durante la
niñez y adolescencia. Por ende, se vuelve fundamental estudiar cómo el contexto
de estimulación en el hogar, específicamente el uso de pantallas, se asocia con
estas habilidades importantes en los primeros años de vida. Si bien existen
investigaciones sobre las asociaciones del uso de pantallas con habilidades
cognitivas tempranas, son escasas las que indagan este fenómeno en el ámbito
latinoamericano, y nulas las que lo realizaron en el actual contexto de
pandemia. El objetivo de esta investigación fue describir el uso de medios
electrónicos (i. e., TV, teléfono celular y tablet) y su asociación con el
lenguaje y los hitos del desarrollo en los primeros años de vida. Los participantes
fueron 253 cuidadores primarios de infantes de Latinoamérica (68.8 % de
Argentina, y 31.2 % de otros países de la región) con edades entre 2 y 48 meses
(M = 30.17 meses, SD = 10.82, femenino = 124), reclutados vía web a través de
muestreo no probabilístico de tipo intencional y por bola de nieve. Se
utilizaron los reportes de los padres sobre el uso de medios electrónicos (i.
e., tiempo de uso, edad de inicio y tipo de contenido), los hitos del
desarrollo motor y del lenguaje, el Inventario de Desarrollo de Habilidades
Comunicativas (CDI) (i. e., densidad léxica y uso de oraciones) y la Encuesta
Permanente de Hogares (EPH) para evaluar las variables sociodemográficas. Los
resultados mostraron que, en promedio, los infantes comenzaron a usar pantallas
durante el primer año de vida en un promedio de una hora por día, con la
televisión como el medio más utilizado y la visualización de videos la
actividad que más realizaban. El número de horas de uso de dispositivos se
asoció positivamente con la densidad léxica y el uso de oraciones: a más horas
de uso, mayor cantidad de palabras y oraciones reportadas. Además, cuanto más
tardía era la edad de inicio de uso de dispositivos, más bajas eran las
puntuaciones en los informes lingüísticos e hitos posteriores del desarrollo
motor y del lenguaje. Los tamaños del efecto para estas asociaciones fueron de
bajos a moderados, lo que indica que otras variables podrían estar
contribuyendo al desarrollo de estas habilidades cognitivas. No se encontraron
diferencias en las capacidades motrices y del lenguaje en función del tipo de
contenido del dispositivo. Tampoco se observaron diferencias en el uso de
pantallas en función del género de los infantes, la edad y las variables
sociodemográficas, lo que podría indicar, tal como demostraron investigaciones
anteriores, que durante el aislamiento por COVID-19 el uso de los medios
electrónicos fue constante, independientemente del contexto del hogar. Los
resultados indican que el uso excesivo de pantallas podría afectar, tanto de forma
positiva como negativa, algunas habilidades tempranas, aunque es necesario
investigar el contexto en el que se utilizan. Específicamente, para futuras
investigaciones sería importante: (a) trabajar con díadas de
cuidadores-infantes, para evaluar cómo los comportamientos parentales moderan
las asociaciones entre uso de pantallas y cognición infantil; (b) realizar
estudios longitudinales; y (c) evaluar estas variables en diferentes provincias
del país y regiones de Latinoamérica, para estudiar cómo las idiosincrasias
culturales modulan el uso de estos dispositivos.
Palabras
clave: medios electrónicos, hitos del desarrollo, lenguaje, cuidadores,
infantes.
Introduction
From the first years of life, development is modulated by a
variable environment indirect to the infant's life (e. g.,
culture, access to education, public policies), within an immediate context (e. g., stimulation at home, parental styles). During
development, these different levels of analysis are interrelated (Bronfenbrenner, 1986; Vygotsky, 1929) and contribute to
differences in infant's cognitive development from the first years of life (Gago Galvagno et al., 2019, 2020a, 2020b) suggesting these factors are a
very important issue to consider when promoting public policies that favor
ontogenetic development (Yoshikawa et al., 2018). The purpose of this study is
to examine how a particular element of the immediate context related to
stimulation at home –electronic media use– relates to cognitive development in
developmental milestones in language, motor skills and sociodemographic during
the first 4 years of life.
Language and motor skills during the first years
Developmental milestones in early childhood refer to
foundational acquisitions. These are explained as the progress of each infant
in different areas and describe observable behaviors in the daily life of a
child (Pauen, 2012). During
development, the acquisition of the different milestones is not carried out in
a rigid way. On the contrary, there is a wide variation in the time that each
new skill is achieved that is impacted by the genetic and the social environment
(Arteaga et al., 2017; Bedford et al., 2016).
Meeting each developmental milestone enables children to perform
progressively more complex and sequential activities. The early acquisition of
both motor and language developmental milestones are essential, since they are
associated with greater achievements in early cognitive skills and later
academic performance, and their delay could be related to the presence of
developmental disorders (Arnett et al.,
2020; Murray et al., 2007; Taanila et al., 2005). Early motor
milestones are typically focused on gross and fine motor development; gross
motor development is comprised of the control of the head, torso, legs, and
movement, including balance, jumping, throwing and catching. Fine motor
development is comprised of hand-body coordination, and holding and
manipulating objects.
Regarding language, verbal communication can be one of the most
important developmental milestones for parents, with children’s first word
usually spoken at the end of the first year of life. Babies usually start by
saying single words, then two-word sentences, and later three-word sentences,
until finally they can form more complex sentences (Cohen & Billard, 2018; Lahrouchi & Kern, 2018). Children
acquire around five words daily. Around two years of age, the linguistic
explosion takes place, in which children pronounce around fifty words, though
there is variation in the time and extent of this explosion (Bates & Carnevale, 1993; Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015).
Several researchers have dedicated themselves to identifying the
content of babies’ first words and the mechanisms by which they are acquired.
For example, the first words of babies are typically nouns with some arguing
that early noun learning, compared to verbs, is a universal feature of human
language (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2015).
The vocabulary and acquisition of grammar rules increases continuously after
the third year (Navarro et al., 2017).
Syntactic development refers to the rules of language, organization of words,
formation of ideas, meaningful sentences and understanding of what is said.
Further development in syntax (i. e., related to
grammatical rules), semantic meaning, and pragmatic understanding continue to
develop across early childhood and is thought to emerge and develop within the
interactive communicative context between children and their environment
–especially tied to communication with adults (Gonzales, 2014; Tomasello, 2000).
Use of screens and associations with language and motor
skills
Electronic media has become an ever-present element within
children’s indirect and immediate environments. The Argentinean Association of
Pediatrics said in 2011 that 90 % of parents reported their children were using
some form of touchscreen and have their own device by the age 3. One third of
infants use electronic media before they even start walking (Melamuda & Waisman, 2019). During the
COVID-19 quarantine, the use of screens in Argentina grew by more than 50 % for
both adults, infants and children (Sociedad
Argentina de Pediatría, 2020; Picco et al., 2020) and the American Academy of Pediatrics (2020)
stopped advising against its use with infants under two years of age to focus
more on factors involving the child, context and content.
In a descriptive parent-report study in children aged 6 to 36
months old, 69.2 % of parents allowed infants to use screens. The mean age of
first use was 11 months and 72.2 % of infants and children used touchscreen
devices 2-3 times a week or more. Consumption time increased with age and male
infants used more screen games than females. The most common reasons parents
cited for touchscreen use in infants were to learn (73.9 %) and entertain (66.3
%). Finally, parents had a positive attitude towards the use of these devices
for educational purposes at an early age (Pempek
& McDaniel, 2016). Thus, this research found that most parents and
caregivers do not comply with the recommendations of national and international
health organizations (American Academy of
Pediatrics, 2020), although it is important to note that screen use is not
consistent across all families. For instance, less recreational screen
consumption is associated with higher levels of mother education, likely also
related to facilitating or promoting certain family and environmental dynamics
(Melamuda & Waisman, 2019; Stienwandt et al., 2020). However, it is
important to note more work and consideration of other factors and context is
needed, as other studies show conflicting results suggesting parents with a
higher education had children engage in more screen time during quarantine due
to COVID-19 (Aguilar-Farias et al., 2021;
Jauregui et al., 2021).
There have also been a number of studies examining the links
between screen time use and milestones in language and motor development, and
results are equivocal. With respect to language there has been some work
showing negative associations with screen use. Zimmerman et al. (2007)
evaluated a total of 1 008 parents of toddlers between 2 to 24 months, who were
surveyed by telephone. They were asked questions about child and parent
demographics, parent-child interactions, and viewing of various types of TV and
DVD/video content. Parents were also asked to complete the MacArthur-Bates
Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). Results indicated that in children 8
to 16 months of age, each hour per day of DVD/video viewing was associated with
a 16.99-point decrease in CDI score. However, among children 17 to 24 months,
there were no associations between any exposure to media and CDI scores.
Other work has shown null or even suggest positive associations
between language and motor milestones and screen use. In a study that examined
association between the use of tablets and the achievement of motor milestones
and linguistic development of children between 6 and 36 months it was observed
that the number of users of touchscreens, as well as the time of use, increased
with infants’ age. Between 6 and 12 months, infants’ use was approximately 9
minutes per day and toddlers between 2 and 3 years of age used approximately 44
minutes per day. Although this study showed that the use of touchscreens
increases rapidly during the first 3 years of life (Cristia & Seidl, 2015), no
associations were found between the use of touchscreens and gross motor and
language milestones, though there was a negative association between the age of
initiation of use of the first device (specifically screen scrolling), and
achievements in fine motor skills (Bedford
et al., 2016). In another study with a sample of infants from Chile, no
associations were found between the amount of time spent on technological
screens and receptive and expressive language scores during the first three
years of life (Alarcón et al., 2018). Research has even suggested a positive
association between screen use and language and motor development. For
instance, Terras & Ramsay (2016)
suggest that, at an early age, touchscreens allow children to explore and get
in touch with various content, even before saying their first words. It was
observed that children who had not yet developed their motor skills and were
not able to turn on a TV or choose which programs to watch, used mobile devices
to watch videos, interactive games and other entertainment (Terras & Ramsay, 2016).
Currently, the use of touch devices in childhood faces a
dilemma. On the one hand, the promotion of digital technology in children has
evidence that its use prepares them for the future, and this is reinforced in
times of the pandemic (Aguilar-Farias et
al., 2021; Ramsay, 2016). On the other hand, pediatric organizations
advocate the minimal use of mobile devices by young children as a result of
concerns about the effects on physical, cognitive, emotional, social,
well-being, and developmental health (Desmurget
& Harlé, 2012). The data provided shows that society has been
incorporating touchscreens in its daily life, and this does not exclude
children and newborns. However, the Argentine Association of Psychiatry (AAP)
has recommended that children under 2 years of age do not use this type of
devices. This is because the possibility of receiving commercial advertising or
exposure to violent or pornographic content. By participating in the networks
without any type of supervision, they can provide inappropriate information, be
subjected to grooming or harassment and, in turn, can be actors of these same
actions (Melamuda & Waismanb, 2019).
Present study
There is a clear need for better evidence to support
psychologists and educators in the role and use of screens during early
childhood. This research is important at a theoretical, practical, and social
level and can lead to a better understanding of how electronic media, so
present in the daily lives of most children today, relates to development. The
objectives of the following research were to: (a) describe the age of
initiation of the electronic media and the hours of daily use by infants from 2
to 48 months; (b) study the relationship between the age of initiation, the
time of use of screens and the acquisition of the motor and language
milestones; (c) associate electronic media use and sociodemographic variables.
A presence of use of screens early in this age range that increased with age, a
negative correlation between time of use of touchscreens with language measures
(i. e., lexical density and use of sentences) and
acquisition of developmental milestones, and greater amount of screen use time
and earlier initiation in families with a lower educational level and with less
independent types of occupations were expected. However, given the mixed
results it was also possible that this research would not find relations, and
this was meant to provide more data in this area.
Method
Participants
The participants in this study were 310 primary caregivers; 56
were excluded because their infants were older than 48 months. The final sample
consisted of 253 primary caregivers of children from 2 to 48 months (M = 30.17
months, SD = 10.82, female = 124), contacted via social media.
The type of sampling was non-probabilistic, intentional, and
snowballing. The study was carried out in Argentina, since the families lived
in this country. Of the total, 68.8 % children were born in Argentina, and 31.2
% were foreign nationals, born in Peru, Mexico, Colombia, Chile, Costa Rica,
and Bolivia. As for caregivers, 73 % were born in Argentina and the rest were
from other Latin American countries. All families had Spanish as their native
language. Of the total number of caregivers who completed the survey, the
majority (n = 230) were the mothers of the infants.
Regarding the sociodemographic variables, the infants’ parents
had an average level of higher education and were professionals (see Table 1).
To carry out the inferential analysis with the CDI inventory and
the development milestones, a sample of 171 participants was used (M = 32.26
months, SD = 8.93, female = 87); 139 cases were excluded from the final sample
because the language measurements were not for infants younger than 12 months
(n = 20), older than 48 months (n = 56) and because of prematurity (n = 63).
Measures
Permanent Household Survey (EPH, INDEC, 2018). Information was collected
on the nationality, age and gender of the child, as well as the educational
level (from 1 = incomplete primary school to 10 = complete postgraduate) and profession (1 = unemployed, 2 = housekeeper,
3 = not qualified, 4 = operator,
5 = employee, 6 = technician,
7 = professional) of the mother and father.
Questionnaire on the use of ad-hoc screens. Information was
collected on what type of touchscreens the infants were using. These included
tablet, TV, and cell phones. It was also asked how many hours per day they used
each of these devices (1 = does not use, 2 = between 0 and 1 hour per day, 3 = between
1 and 2 hours per day, 4 = between 2 and 3 hours
per day, 5 = between 3 and 4 hours per day,
6 = more than 4 hours per day), age of initiation
of different activities (i. e., screen scrolling,
viewing a video, play with a device, watch photos) (1 = between
0 and 5 months, 2 = between 6 and 11 months,
3 = between 12 and 18 months, 4 = between 19 and 25 months, 5 = between
26 and 36 months, 6 = does not use) and what
content they consumed (i. e., music, YouTube,
cartoons, news, interactive games, photos, reading books/stories). A total
score of Age of initiation and Hours of use was calculated by the average of
the starting age of the different activities of the screens and the hours of
use of the different screens respectively.
Developmental milestones (Bedford
et al., 2016). To assess developmental milestones, critical milestones from
the motor and language domains were chosen. The seven questions were as
follows: “At what age did the infant…” and data from motor and language
milestones were used: “sat without support” and “walked independently”, “picked
up a small object with a clamp, that is, with his thumb and forefinger”,
“stacked at least three small blocks or other small objects”, “said his first
word”, “said two or more words together”, “made a whole sentence, meaningful”.
To reply, the caregiver had to indicate their response on a Likert scale, with
age ranges (0 = between 0 to 5 months, 1 = between 6 and 11 months, 2 = between
12 and 18 months, 3 = between 19 and 25 months,
4 = between 26 and 36 months, 5 = still not performed). Variables of motor and language
developmental milestones were generated by the average of each development
milestone.
Communicative Development Inventory Form II (Resches et al., 2021). This inventory
evaluates the development of language in children through the reporting of a
significant caregiver. It is made up of two inventories. Part 1 (CDI 1, lexical
density) measures children’s use of words. It includes a vocabulary list of 23
semantic categories with 699 items. Parents reported the amount of words their
children know. Part 2 (CDI 2, sentence use) inquires about the way in which the
infant uses language, specifically about the evocation of past and future
events, places or people that are not present, detaching language from its
immediate context (symbolic competence). Five questions with examples were
asked with 3 options each (0 = not yet, 1 = sometimes, 2 = many times),
scoring a total on a scale of 0 to 10 points: (1) “Does the child talk about
past situations?”; (2) “Does the child talk about objects or people that are
not present?”; (3) “Does the child talk about things that are going to happen
in the future?”; (4) “Does the child understand when they asked to bring
something from another place?”; (5) “When pointing to or grasping an object,
does the infant say the name of the person to whom the object belongs even
though that person is not present?”. For this sample, a Cronbach’s Alpha of .96
was obtained for CDI 1 and of .84 for CDI 2. About families not from Argentina
(n = 97), a Cronbach’s Alpha of .96 was obtained for CDI 1 and of .88 for CDI
2.
Procedure
The questionnaire was administered virtually. Participants
completed a Google Forms that was shared on Facebook, Instagram, and WhatsApp.
It could be completed from a cell phone, tablet or PC.
Before beginning the evaluation, all participants completed an
informed consent. None received financial compensation, and all completed the
scales individually at home. General objectives of the study were reported, and
participants were invited to participate anonymously, confidentially and
voluntarily to avoid bias in their answers.
All questionnaires were administered in the same order: first
the EPH was administered, then the questionnaire on developmental milestones,
followed by the CDI, and finally the questionnaire on the use of touchscreens.
The data was collected from March 21 to November 11, 2020.
Data analysis
SPSS software version 26 was used. First, a pre-processing of
the data was carried out to evaluate the presence of outliers in the sample. A
descriptive analysis was then carried out on the start and average number of
hours of use of touch devices, lexical density, use of sentences, developmental
milestones, and sociodemographic variables.
The distribution of the variables was calculated using the
Shapiro-Wilk test and the homogeneity of variances with Levene test. Most of
the variables presented a non-normal distribution, so non-parametric tests were
used.
First, a variable description was made. Then, use of electronic
media, CDI scores, developmental milestones, and sociodemographic variables
were correlated using the Spearman Rho test. Device use was compared according
to content type and gender of infants using the Kruskal-Wallis H and
Mann-Whitney U tests respectively. Finally, a multiple linear regression test
was carried out, inserting the variables of device use as input and those of
milestones and language as outcomes.
Results
Description of variables
Table 1 summarizes the main descriptive
statistics. On average, infants began to use screens and carry out activities
with them from the first year of life. Regarding age of initiation of different
activities, only video viewing average occurrence was before the first year.
The latest used content was with tablets, as the activities of zooming to a
screen and playing games with the devices were used for the first time at age
three or not at all. About tablets, 76.8 % (n = 238) of the infants sampled did
not use them, and 42.6 % (n = 132) did not use cell phones. The amount of time
of tablet and cell phone use was less than one hour on average for this sample,
with both variables showing a floor effect reflective of lower scores
indicating lack of use (symmetry > 1.40; West et al., 1999). TV was the most
widely used device with children watching on average more than one hour per day,
and only 12 % of all infants did not use it (n = 37).
Table 1
Descriptive statistics
of measured variables
Measures |
M (SD) |
Median |
95 % IC |
Range |
N |
Age (months) |
30.17 (10.82) |
31.20 |
[28.87, 31.51] |
2-48 |
253 |
> 12 months |
6.90 (3.06) |
7.30 |
[5.42, 8.38] |
2-12 |
19 |
12-24 months |
19.79 (3.12) |
20.50 |
[19.11, 20.84] |
12-24 |
52 |
24-36 months |
30.26 (3.59) |
30.30 |
[29.56, 30.97] |
24-36 |
101 |
36-48 months |
41.82 (3.58) |
42.35 |
[41.03, 42.61] |
36-48 |
81 |
Age of initiation
average |
3.91 (1.06) |
3.85 |
[3.78, 4.04] |
1.28-6 |
253 |
Cell phone use |
3.56 (1.62) |
3 |
[3.36, 3.76] |
1-6 |
253 |
Tablet use |
5.15 (1.41) |
6 |
[4.98, 5.33] |
1-6 |
253 |
Screen scrolling |
3.29 (1.39) |
3 |
[3.12, 3.47] |
1-6 |
243 |
Viewing a video |
2.87 (1.41) |
3 |
[2.69, 3.05] |
1-6 |
243 |
Play with a device |
4.63 (1.53) |
6 |
[4.42, 4.84] |
1-6 |
208 |
Watch photos |
3.30 (1.52) |
3 |
[3.11, 3.50] |
1-6 |
238 |
Zoom to a screen |
4.55 (1.55) |
5 |
[4.34, 4.75] |
1-6 |
218 |
Use time average |
1.99 (0.70) |
2 |
[1.90, 2.08] |
1-4.5 |
253 |
Tablet use time |
1.35 (.74) |
1 |
[1.26, 1.44] |
1-6 |
250 |
Cell phone use time |
1.72 (.82) |
2 |
[1.62, 1.83] |
1-6 |
241 |
TV use time |
2.96 (1.36) |
3 |
[2.79, 3.13] |
1-6 |
250 |
Language |
|||||
Lexical density (CDI 1) |
171.79 (198.80) |
77 |
[141.78, 201.80] |
0-670 |
171 |
Sentence use (CDI 2) |
3.87 (2.43) |
4 |
[3.50, 4.23] |
0-8 |
171 |
Motor development
milestones average |
3.73(0.49) |
3.75 |
[3.65, 3.80] |
1-4.75 |
171 |
Sit unsuported |
4.28 (.55) |
4 |
[4.20, 4.36] |
1-5 |
171 |
Walked independently |
3.08 (.85) |
3 |
[2.95, 3.21] |
0-4 |
171 |
Raised small object |
4.10 (.74) |
4 |
[3.99, 4.21] |
0-5 |
171 |
Stacked two or more
objects |
3.47 (.95) |
4 |
[3.33, 3.62] |
0-5 |
171 |
Language development
milestones average |
2.56 (.98) |
3 |
[2.41, 2.71] |
0-4.33 |
171 |
Said first words |
3.53 (.89) |
4 |
[3.39, 3.66] |
0-5 |
171 |
Said two or more
words |
2.43 (1.30) |
3 |
[2.23, 2.62] |
0-4 |
171 |
Made a meaningful
sentence |
1.74 (1.39) |
2 |
[1.53, 1.95] |
0-4 |
171 |
Sociodemographic
variables |
|||||
Mother education |
6.49(2.10) |
7 |
[6.20, 6.77] |
2-10 |
253 |
Parent education |
5.83(2.19) |
6 |
[5.53, 6.13] |
1-10 |
253 |
Mother occupation |
4.97(2.27) |
6 |
[4.66, 5.28] |
1-7 |
253 |
Parent occupation |
5.41(1.62) |
6 |
[5.19, 5.63] |
1-7 |
253 |
CDI: Communicative Development Inventory.
Associations between screen use and language and motor
milestones
Age of initiation
for screen use and activities carried out with the screens. Results are
summarized in Table 2. For language measured with the CDI,
lexical density was positively associated with a later initiation age of
watching videos. These results indicated that later initiation age with the
activity of watching videos was associated with higher vocabulary use. In the
other hand, lexical density and use of sentences showed a negative relationship
with tablet use and zoom to a screen, indicating that a later initiation age
with tablets and zooming on a screen was associated with lower language
measures reports by parents.
Regarding both motor and language development milestones, the
age at which the developmental milestones were reached was negatively
associated with the initiation age for the different screens and with
activities linked to them (i. e., moving/touching,
zooming and playing interactive games), with moderate effect sizes (.15 <
rho < .27). In other words, as the age of initiation with screens was
delayed, developmental milestones were acquired earlier according to the
parents’ report.
Screen usage time. Positive
associations were found between the sum of hours of different screens and
language measures, with small effect sizes (.21 < rho < .51). This
suggested that as the number of hours of use of electronic devices increased
the scores on lexical density and sentence use did also. No general
associations were found between each screen usage and the sum of screen hours
with developmental milestones (p > .05). Only with the language milestone of
saying two or more words positive associations were found.
Screen initiation
and usage links to sociodemographic variables. No associations
were found between hours of screens use and age of initiation with respect to
sociodemographic variables (i. e., educational
level and type of occupation of the parents), and neither with respect to the
content (i. e., music, YouTube, cartoons, news,
interactive games, photos, reading books/stories) they consumed using the
Kruskall-Wallis H test (p > .05). Finally, a Mann Whitney U test was applied
to compare the number of hours of screen use based on gender. No evidence was
found that suggested that boys and girls used these devices differently in
terms of time (p > .05).
Table 2
Associations between
the variables corresponding to CDI, use of touch devices and development milestones
Language CDI |
Developmental
milestones |
||||||||||||||
Lexical density |
Sentence use |
Motor develop.
milestone avg. |
Language develop.
milestone avg. |
Sit unsupport |
Walk independent |
Raise small object |
Stack 2+ objects |
Say first word |
Say 2+ words |
Made sentence |
|||||
Age of initiation |
|||||||||||||||
Avg. across all
devices |
-.064 |
-.145 |
-.359** |
-.414** |
-.193* |
-.121 |
-.275** |
-.275** |
-.165* |
-.429** |
-.377** |
||||
Cell phone use |
-.014 |
-.120 |
-.217** |
-.238** |
-.079 |
-.060 |
-.227** |
-.164* |
-.159* |
-.278** |
-.159* |
||||
Tablet use |
-.181* |
-.176* |
-.060 |
-.185* |
-.088 |
-.008 |
-.078 |
-.081 |
-.073 |
-.248** |
-.164* |
||||
Screen scrolling |
.106 |
.037 |
-.245** |
-.247** |
-.166* |
-.047 |
-.239** |
-.117 |
-.086 |
-.280** |
-.231** |
||||
Viewing a video |
.163* |
.058 |
-.277** |
-.154* |
-.130 |
-.116 |
-.242** |
-.207** |
-.095 |
-.226** |
-.114 |
||||
Playing with a
device |
-.096 |
-.080 |
-.212* |
-.509** |
-.051 |
-.090 |
-.181 |
-.151 |
-.112 |
-.464** |
-.534** |
||||
Watch photos |
.124 |
-.023 |
-.364** |
-.208** |
-.170* |
-.130 |
-.273** |
-.281** |
-.093 |
-.254** |
-.165* |
||||
Zoom to a screen |
-.185* |
-.178* |
-.390** |
-.396** |
-.226** |
-.145 |
-.220** |
-.300** |
-.103 |
-.390** |
-.388** |
||||
Usage time |
|||||||||||||||
Avg. across all
devices |
.236** |
.275** |
.053 |
.073 |
.036 |
.064 |
-.088 |
.032 |
-.067 |
.095 |
.087 |
||||
Tablet |
.226** |
.211** |
-.026 |
.083 |
.027 |
-.093 |
.011 |
.051 |
-.009 |
.172* |
.066 |
||||
Cell phone |
.153* |
.206** |
.011 |
.133 |
.041 |
-.005 |
-.047 |
-.047 |
.027 |
.167* |
.089 |
||||
TV |
.129 |
.179* |
.049 |
-.009 |
.008 |
.110 |
-.125 |
.019 |
-.100 |
-.044 |
.044 |
||||
CDI: Communicative Development Inventory* p < .05**
p < .01
Contribution of screen use to infant cognition
Three multiple regressions were performed on lexical density,
sentence use, and motor and language developmental milestones, inserting the
age of initiation and the sum of screen time averages as predictors. Table 3 summarizes the results of the regressions. For the
four dependent variables, the general model was significant.
Table 3
Prediction of lexical
density, sentence use, and developmental milestones through the use of screens
F |
B |
Beta |
R2 |
||
Lexical density |
3.491 |
.040* |
|||
Usage time |
53.53 |
.191* |
|||
Age of initiation |
-4.918 |
-.025 |
|||
Use of sentences |
6.764 |
.075** |
|||
Usage time |
.721 |
.210** |
|||
Age of initiation |
-.295 |
-.123 |
|||
Motor development
milestones |
10.07 |
.107** |
|||
Usage time |
-.022 |
-.032 |
|||
Age of initiation |
-.163 |
-.335** |
|||
Language development
milestones |
14.08 |
.144** |
|||
Usage time |
-.006 |
-.004 |
|||
Age of initiation |
-.371 |
-.380** |
* p < .05**
p < .01
Screens usage time positively predicted the language outcomes,
indicating that as the time of screen usage increased, so did the lexical
density and the use of sentences. About age of initiation for the screen
devices and activities, they negatively predicted motor and language
development milestones, with moderate effect sizes. This indicates that as the
age of initiation increased, developmental milestones were acquired earlier
according to the parents’ report.
Discussion
The objective of the present research was to describe the
initiation and hours of use for electronic devices by infants aged 2 to 48
months, and to associate these variables with developmental milestones,
language, and sociodemographic variables. The TV was found to be the most used
device and watching videos was the activity executed the most. On average
infants began to use screens after their first year of life and their use time
was on average one hour per day, except for the TV which was higher.
Surprisingly, a positive association between screen usage hours and lexical
density and sentence use parent reports was found, although the effect sizes
were small. Further, it is important to note that some age of screen and
activities use initiation (i. e., tablet use and
zooming on a screen) were negatively associated with language, indicating that
delaying screen usage was associated with low language report scores, except
for watching videos, where the longer it was delayed, the better vocabulary
reported scores were obtained. In addition, negative associations were found
between the age of initiation of screen use and motor and language
developmental milestones with moderate effect sizes for this age range: as the
age of initiation with screens was delayed, developmental milestones were acquired
earlier according to the parents’ report. No associations were found between
developmental milestones and language outcomes scores with the type of content
that infants consumed on the screens. Also, no associations between screens and
sociodemographic variables were found (i. e.,
infants’ gender, age, and parental educational level and occupation). Results
demonstrate several surprising relations suggesting that age of initiation and
usage may not always relate to negative outcomes in language, and suggest more
work is needed in this area of research. But the age of initiation could
negative relate with the age that different motor and language development
milestones are acquired.
Regarding the descriptive data, although use time was on average
low (one hour) for the electronic media measure, their age of initiation was
early (from the first year of life), which goes against what the different
pediatric associations recommend during early childhood (Asociación Argentina
de Pediatría, 2019; Melamuda &
Waisman, 2019; Waisman et al., 2018).
These results replicate those carried out in samples from Argentina (Waisman et
al., 2018) and the United States (McClane & Pempek, 2015), where it was
found that in this age range children used devices at least one hour per day.
This amount of use may also be reinforced by quarantine, which could lead
parents to use technological devices as a means of regulating or appeasing
infants during their work routines (Chauhan
et al., 2021; Ribner & McHarg,
2021). On the other hand, it is necessary to highlight that the tablet was
hardly used at home, and the device that was used the most was TV. This could
be because TV offer is more varied than other devices, and, at the same time,
it is the only one that does not require active contact and interaction on the
part of the infant, which facilitates its use (Melamuda & Waisman, 2019; Ribner & McHarg, 2021).
Regarding the links between screen use and language, these
results surprisingly demonstrate that the amount of time of media use was
associated with higher scores in both lexical density and use of sentences,
with low effect sizes. Similar positive results were found with the language
development milestone of saying two or more words. There are several reasons
for these results. For one, results have shown that when children are younger,
they require the presence of a caregiver when consuming the screens (Nabi & Krcmar, 2016). A higher
relation between screen use and language could be because a caregiver was
present during the interaction with the screens. Results could also be because
caregivers reported a greater extent of watching videos, which could promote
interaction with adults or expand their lexical density (Ribner & McHarg, 2021; Terras & Ramsay, 2016). It is also
necessary to highlight that although usage time of devices was associated with
better language outcomes, age of initiation with tablet and zooming on a screen
was negatively related. This could be because these are activities that require
the presence of an adult and an active interaction with the children (Bedford et al., 2016). This is reinforced
by the correlation found between a later age of initiation watching videos and
higher reported language scores. This could be because watching videos on
screens in early childhood is a passive activity, and promotes the absence of
interactions with adults, or through books and traditional toys, which
reinforces passivity in the infant (Kostyrka-Allchorne
et al., 2017; Waisman et al., 2018).
It may be useful for future works to consider how time engaged with screens
relates to other interactions at home and how this may change across age.
Regarding developmental milestones, although no general
associations were found with screen use time there were relations to age of
initiation. Specifically, when children initiated screen use later in life,
developmental milestones were acquired earlier. These negative associations of
screen consumption and motor and language milestones could be interpreted with
the displacement hypothesis (Strasburger
et al., 2012), which states that the time a child spends engaged with a
screen limits the time they have to do other activities, leading to reduced
physical activity or face-to-face communication (Ribner & McHarg, 2021). On the other
hand, no associations were found between these developmental milestones and usage
time, which aligns with Bedford et al.’s
(2016) neutral effects but goes against their positive results with fine
motor skills, suggesting that the screens could involve the infants’ motor
movement and their language in an exchange with other adults.
Finally, no associations were found between the use of devices
and sociodemographic variables such as the educational level and occupation of
the parents and the age and gender of the infants. This lack of associations
could be interpreted from the homogeneity of the parent’s sample based on its
characteristics. On the other hand, the equal use of screens according to the
infant’s gender is consistent with other studies (Lin et al., 2020; McHarg et al., 2020). However, the lack
of associations with the age of infants is contradictory with other results
found (McHarg et al., 2020; Melamuda & Waisman, 2019) and
counterintuitive. This may be since on average the use of technologies was low
for this sample due to the age range. In turn, during the COVID-19 quarantine,
the use of screens was present independently of the age of the children (Chauhan et al., 2021; Ribner & McHarg, 2021), which could
make the associations with age disappear.
Conclusion
This research shows the importance of continued research on this
topic since most of the children in the sample use some type of touch device,
and they use them an average of one hour per day. Infants likely start to use
screens early in life and the amount of time of use of electronic media is
approximately one hour per day because: (1) the community is not aware of the
possible negative effects that these devices can generate on cognitive
development, because of the average time that children spent with the screens;
(2) as a means of regulate and distract infants; (3) due to the quarantine
condition derived from the COVID-19 pandemic.
However, affirming that the use of screens in early childhood is
associated with the acquisition of developmental and language milestones
represents the first step in the study of this issue in Argentina and other
Latin American countries. Although these results are not consistent with
previous research on the subject, which states that there is no evidence to support
a negative association between the age of initiation of use of the first touch
device and developmental and language milestones (Bedford et al., 2016; Melamuda & Waisman, 2019), it is
essential to highlight that these investigations were not carried out within
the local environment. Based on the results obtained, interventions can be
generated within the area of clinical psychology in early childhood, giving
guidance to primary caregivers and generating public policies within the
educational field.
Limitations and future directions
The present study presented a series of limitations. One of them
was that the data was collected through parental reports, which could bias the
results obtained. In turn, the type of sampling was non-probabilistic,
therefore, the results cannot be generalized. In addition, the questionnaire
was not directly validated for the small part of the evaluated sample that was
not from Argentina. Thirdly, there was a lack of measurement of parental
activity during the infant’s screen consumption. Finally, having carried out a
cross-sectional study does not allow us to visualize the development
trajectories of these skills in each child.
For future research, it would be beneficial to expand the sample
and recruit participants from different provinces of Argentina and other Latin
American countries. Similarly, it would be recommended to carry out a
probabilistic sampling, based on a direct measurement of the infants’ behavior.
In turn, measure the interactions of the caregiver-infant dyad during screen
consumption to analyze whether they mediate the use of the devices and their
associations with infant cognitive skills. Finally, carry out a longitudinal
study, to be able to compare the same sample over time and thus observe the
development lines of infants. This would lead, in a second moment, to generate
specific interventions that consider the development of infants and the
incidence of social and individual factors to promote their cognition in the
first years of life.
References
Aguilar-Farias, N.,
Toledo-Vargas, M., Miranda-Marquez, S., Cortinez-O'Ryan, A., Cristi-Montero,
C., Rodriguez-Rodriguez, F., ... & del Pozo Cruz, B. (2021). Sociodemographic
predictors of changes in physical activity, screen time, and sleep among
toddlers and preschoolers in chile during the covid-19 pandemic. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public
Health, 18(1), 176-189. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010176
American Academy of
Pediatrics (2020). What do we really know about kids and screens? Monitor on Psychology, 51(3), 42-51.
American Academy of
Pediatrics and Council on Communications and Media. (2016). Media and Young
Minds. Pediatrics, 138(5), e20162591. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2591
Arnett, A. B., Beighley,
J. S., Kurtz‐Nelson, E. C., Hoekzema, K., Wang, T., Bernier, R. A., &
Eichler, E. E. (2020). Developmental predictors of cognitive and adaptive
outcomes in genetic subtypes of autism spectrum disorder. Autism
Research, 13(10), 1659-1669. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2385
Bates, E., &
Carnevale, G. F. (1993). New directions in research on language development.
Developmental review, 13(4), 436-470. https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1993.1020
Bates, E., Benigni,
L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in
infancy. Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-081540-1.50011-9
Bedford, R., Saez de
Urabain, I. R., Cheung, C. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Smith, T. J. (2016).
Toddlers’ fine motor milestone achievement is associated with early touchscreen
scrolling. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1108., https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01108
Bronfenbrenner, U.
(1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research
perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723
Chauhan, N., Patra,
S., Bhargava, R., Srivastava, C., Gujar, K. V., Gupta, N., & Seshadri, S.
(2021). Exposure to Smartphone and Screen media in Children and Adolescents and
COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Indian Association for Child
and Adolescent Mental Health, 17(2), 219-229.
Cohen, L., &
Billard, A. (2018). Social babbling: The emergence of symbolic gestures and
words. Neural Networks, 106, 194-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2018.06.016
Cristia, A., &
Seidl, A. (2015). Parental reports on touch screen use in early childhood. PloS One, 10(6). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128338
Desmurget, M. &
Harlé, B. (2012). Effets de l’exposition chronique aux écrans sur le développement
cognitif de l’enfant. Archives de Pédiatrie, 19(7),
772-776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arcped.2012.04.003
Elgier, A. M.,
Galvagno, L. G., Clerici, G., Tortello, C., & Azzolini, S. C. (2017).
Seguimiento del gesto de señalar y de la mirada en estadios tempranos del
desarrollo. Apuntes de Ciencia & Sociedad, 7(1),
1-14. https://doi.org/10.18259/acs.2017003
Gago Galvagno, L.
G., De Grandis, M. C., Clerici, G. D., Mustaca, A. E., Miller, S. E., &
Elgier, A. M. (2019). Regulation during the second year: Executive function and
emotion regulation links to joint attention, temperament and social
vulnerability in a Latin American sample. Frontiers in
Psychology, 10, 1473. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01473
Gago Galvagno, L.
G., De Grandis, M. C., Jaume, L. C., & Elgier, A. M. (2020b). Home
environment and its contribution to early childhood regulatory capabilities. Early Child Development and Care, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2020.1796655
Gago-Galvagno, L.,
Clerici, G., & Elgier, A. (2020a). ¿Contribuyen los subsidios estatales al
desarrollo cognitivo temprano? La Asignación Universal por Hijo en el contexto
argentino. Psicología UNEMI, 4(7), 8-20. https://doi.org/10.29076/issn.2602-8379vol4iss7.2020pp8-20p
Gutnick, A. L.,
Robb, M., Takeuchi, L., & Kotler, J. (2010). Always
connected: The new digital habits of young children. The Joan Ganz
Cooney Center at Sesame Workshop.
Haslett, B.,
Bonniwell, H. B., & Samter, W. (1997). Children
communicating: The first 5 years. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Hirsh-Pasek, K.,
Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., ... &
Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income
children’s language success. Psychological science, 26(7),
1071-1083. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615581493
INDEC (2018). Acceso y uso de tecnologías de la información y la comunicación.
Buenos Aires: EPH. https://www.indec.gob.ar/uploads/informesdeprensa/mautic_05_213B13B3593A.pdf
Jauregui, A., Salvo,
D., Aguilar-Farias, N., & Okely, A. (Preprint). Movement Behaviors during
COVID-19. A Survey among Children under Five Years of Latin American Origin or
Descent from Chile, Mexico, and the US. Research Square.
https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-140628/v1
Kostyrka-Allchorne,
K., Cooper, N. R., & Simpson, A. (2017). The relationship between
television exposure and children’s cognition and behaviour: A systematic
review. Developmental Review, 44, 19-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.12.002
Lahrouchi, M., &
Kern, S. (2018). From babbling to first words in language acquisition:
longitudinal two-case studies. Canadian Journal of
Linguistics/Revue Canadienne de Linguistique, 63(4), 493-526. https://doi.org/10.1017/cnj.2018.6
Lin, H. P., Chen, K.
L., Chou, W., Yuan, K. S., Yen, S. Y., Chen, Y. S., & Chow, J. C. (2020).
Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated with emotional and behavioral
problems, but not language delay, in toddlers. Infant
Behavior and Development, 58, 101424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101424
McHarg, G., Ribner,
A. D., Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2020). Screen time and executive
function in toddlerhood: A longitudinal study. Frontiers
in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.570392
Melamuda, A., & Waismanb,
I. (2019). Pantallas: discordancias entre las recomendaciones y el uso real. Archivos Argentinos de Pediatría, 117(5), 349-351. https://doi.org/10.5546/aap.2019.349
Miller, S. E., &
Marcovitch, S. (2015). Examining executive function in the second year of life:
coherence, stability, and relations to joint attention and language. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 101-125. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038359
Moreno-Carmona, N.
D., Marín-Cortés, A., Cano-Bedoya, V. H., Sanabria-González, J. A.,
Jaramillo-Suarez, Á. M., & Ossa-Ossa, J. C. (2021). Mediaciones parentales
y uso de internet por niños, niñas y adolescentes colombianos. Interdisciplinaria, 38(2), 275-290. https://doi.org/10.16888/interd.2021.38.2.18
Murray, G. K.,
Jones, P. B., Kuh, D., & Richards, M. (2007). Infant developmental
milestones and subsequent cognitive function. Annals of
Neurology, 62(2), 128-136. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.21120
Nabi, R. L., &
Krcmar, M. (2016). It takes two: the effect of child characteristics on US parents’
motivations for allowing electronic media use. Journal of
Children and Media, 10(3), 285-303. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2016.1162185
Navarro, J., Pérez,
F., & Arteaga, M. (2017). Vínculos tempranos:
Transformaciones al inicio de la vida. Ediciones Universidad Alberto
Hurtado.
Pauen, S., Frey, B.,
& Ganser, L. (2012). Entwicklungs psychologie in den
ersten drei Lebensjahren. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-45742-9_2
Pempek,
T. A., & McDaniel, B. T. (2016). Young children’s tablet use and
associations with maternal well-being. Journal of Child and
Family Studies, 25(8), 2636-2647. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016-0413-x
Resches,
M., Querejeta, M., Cortada, A. K., & Laguens, A. (2021). Adaptación y
normativización del Inventario del Desarrollo Comunicativo Mac Arthur Bates
(CDI-Forma II) al español rioplatense. Revista
Iberoamericana de Psicología: Ciencia y Tecnología, 14(3),
107-120. https://reviberopsicologia.ibero.edu.co/article/view/2038
Ribner, A. D., &
McHarg, G. (2021). Screens across the pond: Findings from longitudinal screen
time research in the US and UK. Infant Behavior and
Development, 63, 101551.
Sociedad Argentina
de Pediatría (2020). Uso de pantallas en tiempos del coronavirus. Archivos Argentinos de Pediatría, Suplemento COVID,
142-144. https://doi.org/10.5546/aap.2018.e186
Stienwandt, S.,
Cameron, E. E., Soderstrom, M., Casar, M. J., Le, C., & Roos, L. E. (2020).
Keeping Kids Busy: Family Factors Associated with Hands-on Play and Screen Time
During the COVID-19 Pandemic. PsyArXiv Preprints.https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/prtyf
Strasburger, V. C.,
Jordan, A. B., & Donnerstein, E. (2012). Children, adolescents, and the
media: health effects. Pediatric Clinics, 59(3),
533-587. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcl.2012.03.025
Taanila, A., Murray,
G. K., Jokelainen, J., Isohanni, M., & Rantakallio, P. (2005). Infant
developmental milestones: a 31-year follow-up. Developmental
Medicine and Child Neurology, 47(9), 581-586. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01207.x
Terras, M. M., &
Ramsay, J. (2016). Family digital literacy practices and children’s mobile
phone use. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01957
Thompson, A. L.,
Adair, L. S., & Bentley, M. E. (2013). Maternal characteristics and
perception of temperament associated with infant TV exposure. Pediatrics, 131(2), e390-e397. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-1224
Tomasello, M.
(2000). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 156-163.
Vygotski, L. S.
(1929). The problem of the cultural development of the child. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 36(3),
415-434. https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1929.10532201
Waisman, I.,
Hidalgo, E., & Rossi, M. L. (2018). Uso de pantallas en niños pequeños en
una ciudad de Argentina. Archivos Argentinos de Pediatría,
116(2), e186-e195. https://doi.org/10.5546/aap.2018.e186
HTML generado a partir
de XML-JATS4R por
Refbacks
- No hay Refbacks actualmente.
Añadir comentario